Explanations for forgetting
Specification: Explanations for forgetting: proactive and retroactive interference and retrieval failure due to absence of cues.
Longāterm memories are not always remembered, and forgetting can occur for different reasons. Psychologists have suggested different explanations for forgetting: proactive interference, retroactive interference and retrieval failure due to the absence of cues.
Interference theories suggest that forgetting is caused by competing memories, either because existing memories interfere with the learning of new information (proactive interference) or because new information interferes with previously learnt information (retroactive interference). Furthermore, forgetting can also occur because information cannot be retrieved, due to insufficient cues to trigger memory.
Proactive interference
Proactive interference occurs when old information stored in longāterm memory (LTM), interferes with the learning of new information. This usually occurs when the new information is similar to the old information. An everyday example of proactive interference is when you get a new mobile phone number: your memory for your old number will disrupt your attempts to remember your new number.
Keppel & Underwood (1962)Ā
Aim: To investigate the effect of proactive interference on LTM.
Method: In an experiment that is very similar to that conducted by Peterson & Peterson (1959), participants were presented with meaningless threeāletter consonant trigrams (for example, THG) at different intervals (3, 6, 9 seconds, etc.) To prevent rehearsal the participants had to count backwards in threes before recalling.
Results: Participants typically remembered the trigrams that were presented first, irrespective of the interval length.
Conclusion: The results suggest proactive interference occurred, as memory for the earlier consonants (which had transferred to LTM) interfered with the memory for new consonants, due to the similarity of the information presented.
Retroactive Interference
Retroactive interference occurs when the learning of new information interferes with the recall of old information from LTM. For example, once you have learned your new mobile number, it is often very difficult to recall your old number.
Ā
Baddeley & Hitch (1977)Ā
Aim: To investigate retroactive interference in everyday memory.
Method: The sample comprised rugby union players who had played every match in the season and players who had missed some games due to injury. The length of time from the start to the end of the season was the same for all players, and players were asked to recall the names of the teams they had played against earlier in the season.
Results: The players who had played the most games forgot proportionately more games than those who had played fewer games due to injury.
Conclusion: Baddeley and Hitch concluded that this was the result of retroactive inference, as the learning of new information (new team names) interfered with the memory of old information (earlier team names).
Evaluating Interference Theories
The results of Baddeley and Hitch are support by other researchers, for example McGeoch and McDonald (1931). In their experiment, participants were given a list of ten adjectives to learn (list A). Once these adjectives were learnt, participants were then given one of six other lists (list B) to learn, which varied in terms of its similarity to the original. McGeoch and McDonald found recall was worse when lists A and B were closest in similarity. This supports the idea of retroactive interference because the more similar the new material is to the previously learnt material, the greater the interference.Ā Ā
Ā
Although interference research (proactive and retroactive) provides an insight into one type of forgetting, it only explains a specific type of forgetting ā memory for similar information. For example, the results of Baddeley and Hitch demonstrate retroactive interference in rugby union players trying to recall team names from earlier in the season and Keppel and Underwood demonstrate proactive interference when trying to learn threeāletter consonant trigrams. Both of these examples highlight interference effects of very similar information and therefore this research is limited in its real world application and are unable to explain forgetting in other situations.Ā
Ā
Furthermore, interference research is often criticised for being artificial and lacking ecological validity. Most of the research examining interference is carried out in a laboratory, for example Keppel and Underwood (1962) and McGeoch and McDonald (1931), while using particularly meaningless stimuli, such as threeāletter consonant trigrams or simple word lists. As a result these findings do not represent everyday examples of interference and are limited in their application to everyday human memory.
Retrieval Failure Due to Absence of Cues
Another type of forgetting occurs when information cannot be retrieved because of insufficient cues to trigger memory. Tulving and Thomson (1973) proposed the encoding specificity principle and argued that memory is most effective when information that was present at the time of coding is also present at the time of retrieval. Furthermore, they suggested that environmental cues and mental cues aid recall. Environmental cues include the room in which you learn information, and mental cues include your emotional state.
Consequently there are two types of retrieval failure due to the absence of cues: 1) contextādependent failure and 2) stateādependent failure. Contextādependent failure occurs when environmental cues are missing and stateādependent failure occurs when an individualās emotional state is different when trying to recall information.Ā Ā
Godden & Baddeley (1975)Ā
Aim: To investigate the effect of contextual cues on recall (i.e. would memory for words learned and recalled in the same environment be better than memory for words learned and recalled in different environments?)
Method: Their sample comprised 18 participants (13 males and 5 females) from a university diving club, who were divided into four conditions: 1) learning words on land and recalling on land; 2) learning words on land and recalling underwater; 3) learning underwater and recalling underwater; and 4) learning underwater and recalling on land. The experiment was a repeated measures design with each participant taking part in all four conditions, over four separate days. In all four conditions participants were presented with 38 words, which they heard twice. After hearing all 38 words the participants were instructed to write all the words they could remember, in any order.
Results: The words learned underwater were better recalled underwater and words learned on land were better recalled on land.
Conclusion: It is, therefore, reasonable to conclude that the environmental cues (context) improve recall.
Evaluating Context-Dependent Forgetting
Although the results suggest that environment cues aid memory recall, Godden & Baddeley didnāt control many other variables. The divers took part in the experiment at different times of the day and at different diving locations. Therefore, each diver would have experienced other contextual cues, which may have affected their memory. Therefore, we are unable to conclude whether the results of Godden & Baddeleyās research is due to the on land/underwater contextual cues, or another contextual cue provided by the different time of day or diving location.
Godden & Baddeley used a repeated measures design, as each diver took part in all four conditions. It is possible that the divers worked out the aim of the experiment and displayed demand characteristics or order effects. By the fourth trial the participants may have demonstrated practice effects where their recall improved as a result of completing the experiment multiple times, or even fatigue effects where their results declined as a result of boredom. A more suitable experimental design would have been independent measures. However, this would have required significantly more participants, which would be difficult to achieve when recruiting trained divers. Furthermore, with a sample of just 18 divers the conclusions drawn should be treated with caution. Additionally, the context examined in their study is extreme and provides little insight into contextādependent forgetting in everyday life.
Godden & Baddeley could also be criticised for breaking ethical guidelines, in particular protection from harm. In their report, they said: āOne diver was nearly run over during an underwater experimental session by an exāarmy, amphibious DUKWā, and therefore more precautions should have been taken to ensure the safety of their participants.Ā
StateāDependent Forgetting
When the emotional state that an individual is in serves as an aid to memory recall, there is a risk that stateādependent forgetting will occur when the same psychological state is not experienced. This is often the case with alcohol intoxication and the absence of accurate memories when sober.Ā
Carter & Cassaday (1998)
Aim: Carter & Cassaday (1998) examined stateādependent forgetting using antiāhistamine drugs. These are typically administered as hay fever relief to sufferers but are also known for their sedating effect. This can make the individual feel drowsy, and therefore not as alert as they would normally be, providing a comparison to everyday nonādrugāinduced behaviour.Ā
Method: Participants were tasked with learning a list of words and excerpts from a text and then asked to recall the information at a later point. There were four conditions in their experiment:Ā
learn the words/text after taking antiāhistamine and recall after taking antiāhistamine
learn the words/text without antiāhistamine and recall without antiāhistamine
learn the words/text after talking antiāhistamine and recall without antiāhistamine drugs
learn the words/text without antiāhistamine and recall after taking the antiāhistamine drugsĀ
Results: In the conditions where the learning and recalling state matched (i.e. after taking the drugs on both occasions or not taking the drugs both occasions) memory was improved. Consequently, when the physiological state of the participants was different recall was significantly poorer.Ā
Conclusion: When the physiological/emotional cues that are present at the time of encoding are missing at the time of retrieval (recall), stateādependent forgetting is likely to occur.Ā
Evaluating StateāDepending Forgetting
There is research support for the effect of stateādependent retrieval failure, which occurs when an individualās emotional state at the time of learning is different to their emotional state at the time of recall. For example, Goodwin et al. (1969), asked male volunteers to remember lists of words when they were either drunk or sober. The participants were then asked to recall the words 24 hours later, in either a drunk or sober state. The results of Goodwin et al. support Godden and Baddeley, as words learned when drunk were better recalled when drunk, and words learnt when sober were better recalled when sober. These results support the idea of stateādependent retrieval failure and demonstrate the power of āstateā on recalling information.Ā
There is research support for stateādependent forgetting using a range of different substances to create an alternative state of consciousness. Darley et al., (1973) researched the impact of marijuana on an individualās recollection. It was found that individuals who were under the influence of marijuana when they put money in a āsafe placeā were less able to recall where this location was once they were no longer under the influence of the drug. This evidence adds weight to the argument that the emotional and physiological state that a person is in at the time of encoding is important at the time of retrieval.Ā
There are issues with determining a cause and effect relationship with retrieval failure as an explanation of forgetting. Nairne (2002) criticised research in this area suggesting that there is merely a correlation between cues present at the time of encoding and cues present at the time of later retrieval. He goes further and suggests that the cues present do not in themselves cause the retrieval failure (or success), but are simply associated with it. This would mean that the cueādependent (context and state) explanations of forgetting due to retrieval failure, and in fact circular in nature rather than linear and psychologists are unable to conclude whether a lack of cues actually causes retrieval failure.